Personal View site logo
Make sure to join PV on Telegram or Facebook! Perfect to keep up with community on your smartphone.
The Making of an Expert
  • Thirty years ago, two Hungarian educators, László and Klara Polgár, decided to challenge the popular assumption that women don’t succeed in areas requiring spatial thinking, such as chess. They wanted to make a point about the power of education. The Polgárs homeschooled their three daughters, and as part of their education the girls started playing chess with their parents at a very young age. Their systematic training and daily practice paid off. By 2000, all three daughters had been ranked in the top ten female players in the world. The youngest, Judit, had become a grand master at age 15, breaking the previous record for the youngest person to earn that title, held by Bobby Fischer, by a month. Today Judit is one of the world’s top players and has defeated almost all the best male players.

    It’s not only assumptions about gender differences in expertise that have started to crumble. Back in 1985, Benjamin Bloom, a professor of education at the University of Chicago, published a landmark book, Developing Talent in Young People, which examined the critical factors that contribute to talent. He took a deep retrospective look at the childhoods of 120 elite performers who had won international competitions or awards in fields ranging from music and the arts to mathematics and neurology. Surprisingly, Bloom’s work found no early indicators that could have predicted the virtuosos’ success. Subsequent research indicating that there is no correlation between IQ and expert performance in fields such as chess, music, sports, and medicine has borne out his findings. The only innate differences that turn out to be significant— and they matter primarily in sports—are height and body size.

    So what does correlate with success? One thing emerges very clearly from Bloom’s work: All the superb performers he investigated had practiced intensively, had studied with devoted teachers, and had been supported enthusiastically by their families throughout their developing years. Later research building on Bloom’s pioneering study revealed that the amount and quality of practice were key factors in the level of expertise people achieved. Consistently and overwhelmingly, the evidence showed that experts are always made, not born. These conclusions are based on rigorous research that looked at exceptional performance using scientific methods that are verifiable and reproducible. Most of these studies were compiled in The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance, published last year by Cambridge University Press and edited by K. Anders Ericsson, one of the authors of this article. The 900-page-plus handbook includes contributions from more than 100 leading scientists who have studied expertise and top performance in a wide variety of domains: surgery, acting, chess, writing, computer programming, ballet, music, aviation, firefighting, and many others.

    The journey to truly superior performance is neither for the faint of heart nor for the impatient. The development of genuine expertise requires struggle, sacrifice, and honest, often painful self-assessment. There are no shortcuts. It will take you at least a decade to achieve expertise, and you will need to invest that time wisely, by engaging in “deliberate” practice— practice that focuses on tasks beyond your current level of competence and comfort. You will need a well-informed coach not only to guide you through deliberate practice but also to help you learn how to coach yourself.

    Read the rest: http://www.coachingmanagement.nl/The%20Making%20of%20an%20Expert.pdf

    Longer and more popular versions:

    http://www.amazon.com/gp/product/B001HD8NZ8

    http://www.amazon.com/Outliers-Story-Success-Malcolm-Gladwell/dp/0316017922

  • 16 Replies sorted by
  • I think the 10,000 hours or there abouts, idea to become masterful rings true in a lot of diciplines, but often the critical information regarding which practices are the best to work on, are difficult to find. Sure enough any deliberate practice will help, but well informed coaches who are willing to pass on their knowledge rather than squirreling it away.. seem elusive. (That's one of the reasons Personal-view stands out:)

    I often hear the cry "Shoot shoot shoot!" It would be great to have some suggestions from the more experienced filmmakers as to how new explorers of the GH2 and filmmaking like myself, could spend their 10,000 hours more effectively.

    Any suggestions greatfully received! :)

  • The Gladwell book -- a popularized version of some current research, as VK notes -- strikes this reader as being full of holes and circular arguments. For example, he cites Bill Gates' early intensive programming experience, satisfying the 10,000 hour rule, but fails to note that Gates' business success didn't have much to do with his programming expertise, any more than Mark Zuckerberg's success depended on extraordinary ability as a coder.

    And if 10,000 hours is the threshold for any reasonably intelligent hard-working person, how can the skills acquired by that effort be considered "world-class"? It may be true that certain professions or activities can be mastered in 10,000 hours with good training, but what exactly does that prove, that we didn't know before?

    The deficiencies of the model become obvious in the arts. There are many, many children who acquire shocking technical proficiency on the classical piano or violin well before the age 10, satisfying the 10,000 hour requirement several times over before they're adults. And yet most of them have no idea what they're playing, any more than you'd expect a 10-year old to write a mature novel. And, in the end, very few of these kids will ever be world-class musicians, even putting aside the difficulties of having a commercially viable career.

    Similarly, people spend lifetimes trying to write their "great book". But there is no such thing as "deliberate practice" for that kind of open-ended endeavor. Writing a coherent book is either relatively easy for any given person (even if the work takes a few years) or will prove utterly impossible, as it does prove for the vast majority of people who try. There are no effective programs of instruction, you either have the capacity for that sort of work, or you don't.

  • Fair points JRD, Gladwell actually actually enumerates other elements that factor in success, such as timing and happenstance. About that great novel? It takes creativity to do that and no one really knows what creativity is and how to measure it. It doesn't register on IQ tests and SAT's. So what are a ya gonna do? Keep writing! Keep painting! Keep filming (but not bushes...)

  • The deficiencies of the model become obvious in the arts. There are many, many children who acquire shocking technical proficiency on the classical piano or violin well before the age 10, satisfying the 10,000 hour requirement several times over before they're adults. And yet most of them have no idea what they're playing, any more than you'd expect a 10-year old to write a mature novel.

    I never saw musical performer who born genius, all of them worked extremely hard. Idea that arts is not affected by practice improvement is absolutely wrong.
    As for children and adults who practice music - many are just forced by smart parent, as most music careers are not really bright.

    About that great novel? It takes creativity to do that and no one really knows what creativity is and how to measure it.

    Firts, you need to define "great novel". Most of the movie scripts and novels level drop in recent years can be directly attributed to lack of life expirience. So, for authors it is extremely important to practice also, generally, it is their sole goal, but it is not about writing on paper, it is more about real life expirience.

  • Nobody disputes that the arts require a tremendous amount of work. But there are tens of thousands of people who put in far more than the 10,000 hour requirement, and with the benefit of high quality formal training, and never succeed. Music conservatories, MFA writing programs and film schools are full of these people.

    Or take the chess-playing Polgar girls. How many kids are trained as chess players by their parents, with the same work and dedication, but remain mediocre? The passage above doesn't say.

    And I would argue that there are musical performers who are born geniuses. They acquire skills at a rate of progress that would astound "normal" people, and which most others couldn't equal in a lifetime, no matter how hard they worked.

  • "Nobody disputes that the arts require a tremendous amount of work. But there are tens of thousands of people who put in far more than the 10,000 hour requirement, and with the benefit of high quality formal training, and never succeed. Music conservatories, MFA writing programs and film schools are full of these people."

    +1

    No two brains on earth are wired the same. Some of us are better at mathematical and memory though processes, some at music, some are dumb... but can throw balls good. And like someone said above... nobody can pinpoint exactly where creativity comes from, but you can't teach it.

    It's all just genetic variations and mutations.

    It's nice to think that we all could accomplish anything we put in the time for, but I just don't believe it works this way. Sure, you can develop skills, but that initial talent... the ability to hear new music in your head, or watch movies that don't exist yet, needs to be there in the first place. It's just like professional sports, most kids try, most kids work hard, but some are just born with better physical makeup.

    In short, using a sports analogy... You can shoot hoops all you want, but it won't make you taller.

  • Nobody disputes that the arts require a tremendous amount of work. But there are tens of thousands of people who put in far more than the 10,000 hour requirement, and with the benefit of high quality formal training, and never succeed. Music conservatories, MFA writing programs and film schools are full of these people.

    If your go in circles for years you are not going anywhere. This is about such people.

    Or take the chess-playing Polgar girls. How many kids are trained as chess players by their parents, with the same work and dedication, but remain mediocre? The passage above doesn't say.

    As one who watched very big amount of kids who had been trained by good trainer I can see that none of them who really worked remained mediocre.

    And I would argue that there are musical performers who are born geniuses. They acquire skills at a rate of progress that would astound "normal" people, and which most others couldn't equal in a lifetime, no matter how hard they worked.

    Many people think such, but it is wrong. As things they misinterpret as genius is sometimes just their family, friends, teachers, things that surrounds them or their expirience in other fields.

    People are not the same. But. Progress, real progress is not defined genetically only. Our mind is amazing thing, and theories that you can not develop it because you are dumb... Leave me alone.
    I saw hundreds and hundreds of students personally, none where dumb, and all who though so was lazy :-)

    No two brains on earth are wired the same. Some of us are better at mathematical and memory though processes, some at music, some are dumb... but can throw balls good. And like someone said above... nobody can pinpoint exactly where creativity comes from, but you can't teach it.

    As I said, brains are complex and interesting things.
    And most interesting thing is that almost all things, that we assume uder term "intelligence", you can develop. If you work hard enough.

    As for creativity. I don't give a fuck. Creativity is just good word used in a bad way.
    I never saw "creative" people who could invent something interesting, give good idea or write good script. But I know bunch of others who work hard and know real life. Sometimes "creative" ones steal ideas and expirience from former ones and call it their archievement.

  • I never saw musical performer who born genius, all of them worked extremely hard. Idea that arts is not affected by practice improvement is absolutely wrong. As for children and adults who practice music - many are just forced by smart parent, as most music careers are not really bright.

    It's true, most music child prodigies don't amount to much, as JRD says, these young kids can often play with technical competence beyond their years, but at the end of the day they're just banging. I Have a 10y/o that's labelled "Musically gifted" and gets a lot of attention and scholarships etc. But if you heard her play, she makes just as many mistakes as any 10 yearold who has been playing a few years; what's different is her playing somehow just sound musical. But, her music will no go anywhere unless she decides to practice like a fiend.

    Firts, you need to define "great novel". Most of the movie scripts and novels level drop in recent years can be directly attributed to lack of life expirience. So, for authors it is extremely important to practice also, generally, it is their sole goal, but it is not about writing on paper, it is more about real life expirience.

    Commercial success doesn't typically define great art IMO. Movie scripts are a particularly bad example because 1) They're all written by committee, even if a single writer is credited, 99% he's made extensive revisions/rewrites based on the whims and judgement of execs, producers, directors, and actors even 2) Screenplays only vaguely qualify as art -- there's a reason there is zero market for screenplays as something engaging to read. So I go with critics as to what defines great art, they're the gatekeepers IMO. Right now the #1 Bestseller is a piece of soft core garbage called "50 Shades of Gray". 100 years from now, people will most likely still be astonished by Chekhov, Melville, or Faulkner.

    I hate to admit it, by I'm loving what V is saying (shhh), to it I'll add 1) Michael Jordan couldn't make is HS basketball team and 2) I grew up next do to a guy who was a good athelete, he was better than I at all sports, but not by a lot really, and I'm purely average. The real difference was his determination to be a baseball player. He didn't make his HS baseball team until his Sr. year. He played 12 years in the majors and made 17 million dollars. Never count yourself out. When in doubt, go with V.

  • There is obviously a mix of abilities in any group of practitioners, and these abilities are subject to a wide range of influences, but there is no substitute for practice. I would prefer students who are willing to put the time in to develop their skills rather than innately tallented students who more often than not, have some awareness of their talents and often find it dificult to break free of the comfort zone of their familiar way of working. If a talented student knows that they can impress with the work they already produce, they often (understandably) keep repeating the same working practices as they know they will produce good work.. this level of achievement is soon surpassed by those students without the innate tallent, but who are willing to develop their skills.

    In my experience the recent increase in numbers of students who expect skills to be absorbed by attending institutional establishments is a worrying trend and is linked to the idea that talent alone will get you where you want to be, rather than understanding that deliberate practice and the need to develop the skills needed to become independent learners are more important than any level of natural skill.

  • @matthere

    Even worse is that current trend is clearly bigger and bigger companies that are more and more "efficient" (in a business meaning). Add here automatization. and globalization And you have big red cross. More and more people expect to do simple, unchanging job, and less and less of such people are required.

  • A very good illustration (the best maybe?) of what VK said can be found in the wonderfull work of Norbert Elias (a great historian/sociologist) : Mozart: Portrait of a Genius.

  • @astraban Sounds interesting, however, just have to point out, Mozart actually was a child prodigy.

  • Well that's the point of the study : take the best example of a "child prodigy" in the history and try to describe the sociological element (education, social environment, etc) who contribute in the making of that strange thing that we call genius. Elias is not that simple (and my english is awful to give justice to his subtility) by saying that genius is a fiction and all it takes to make a mozart is practice and a specific environment that will work for any kid... But his work is a good way to attenuate all the magical aspect that the "genius" concept tend to introduce in the description of the most notable historical figure.

  • Here's a decent summary of Gladwell's ideas for people who don't wanna bother with the book. Interesting that Gladwell himself was a prodigy.

    The Myth of Prodigy and Why it Matters

    By Eric Wargo, Observer Staff Writer

    Judging from his boyish appearance and his voracious curiosity, it’s easy to imagine Malcolm Gladwell as some sort of child prodigy. And he was. But not the way you imagined.

    As a teenager growing up in rural Ontario, the bestselling author of Blink and The Tipping Point was a champion runner, the number-one Canadian runner of his age. He was encouraged to dream of Olympic gold, and indeed was flown to special training camps with the other elite runners of his generation — on the assumption that creating future world-class athletes meant recognizing and nurturing youthful talent.

    Precocity was the subject of Gladwell’s “Bring the Family Address” at this year’s APS Convention, and the account of his own early athletic success served as a springboard. “I was a running prodigy,” he said bluntly. But — and this “but” sounded the theme of his talk to the rapt audience filling the Marquis Marriott’s Broadway Ballroom — being a prodigy didn’t forecast future success in running. After losing a major race at age 15, then enduring other setbacks and loss of interest, Gladwell said, he gave up running for a few years. Taking it up again in college — with the same dedication as before — he faced a disappointing truth: “I realized I wasn’t one of the best in the country … I was simply okay.”

    The fall from childhood greatness to a middling state of “simply okay” is, Gladwell suggested, a recurring theme when the cherished notion of precocity is subjected to real scrutiny.

    “I think we take it as an article of faith in our society that great ability in any given field is invariably manifested early on, that to be precocious at something is important because it’s a predictor of future success,” Gladwell said. “But is that really true? And what is the evidence for it? And what exactly is the meaning and value of mastering a particular skill very early on in your life?”

    There are two ways of answering these questions. One is simply to track the achievements of precocious kids. Gladwell cited a mid-1980s study (Genius Revisited) of adults who had attended New York City’s prestigious Hunter College Elementary School, which only admits children with an IQ of 155 or above. Hunter College was founded in the 1920s to be a training ground for the country’s future intellectual elite. Yet the fate of its child-geniuses was, well, “simply okay.” Thirty years down the road, the Hunter alums in the study were all doing pretty well, were reasonably well adjusted and happy, and most had good jobs and many had graduate degrees. But Gladwell was struck by what he called the “disappointed tone of the book”: None of the Hunter alums were superstars or Nobel- or Pulitzer-prize winners; there were no people who were nationally known in their fields. “These were genius kids but they were not genius adults.”

    A similar pattern emerged when Gladwell examined his own cohort of elite teen runners in Ontario. Of the 15 nationally ranked runners in his age class at age 13 or 14, only one of that group had been a top runner in his running prime, at age 24. Indeed, the number-one miler at age 24 was someone Gladwell had known as one of the poorer runners when they were young — Doug Consiglio, a “gawky kid” of whom all the other kids asked “Why does he even bother?”

    Precociousness is a slipperier subject than we ordinarily think, Gladwell said. And the benefits of earlier mastery are overstated. “There are surprising numbers of people who either start good and go bad or start bad and end up good.”

    Gifted Learning vs. Gifted Doing

    The other way to look at precocity is of course to work backward — to look at adult geniuses and see what they were like as kids. A number of studies have taken this approach, Gladwell said, and they find a similar pattern. A study of 200 highly accomplished adults found that just 34 percent had been considered in any way precocious as children. He also read a long list of historical geniuses who had been notably undistinguished as children — a list including Copernicus, Rembrandt, Bach, Newton, Beethoven, Kant, and Leonardo Da Vinci (“that famous code-maker”). “None of [them] would have made it into Hunter College,” Gladwell observed.

    We think of precociousness as an early form of adult achievement, and, according to Gladwell, that concept is much of the problem. “What a gifted child is, in many ways, is a gifted learner. And what a gifted adult is, is a gifted doer. And those are quite separate domains of achievement.”

    To be a prodigy in music, for example, is to be a mimic, to reproduce what you hear from grown-up musicians. Yet only rarely, according to Gladwell, do child musical prodigies manage to make the necessary transition from mimicry to creating a style of their own. The “prodigy midlife crisis,” as it has been called, proves fatal to all but a handful would-be Mozarts. “Precociousness, in other words, is not necessarily or always a prelude to adult achievement. Sometimes it’s just its own little discrete state.”

    Early acquisition of skills — which is often what we mean by precocity — may thus be a misleading indicator of later success, said Gladwell. “Sometimes we call a child precocious because they acquire a certain skill quickly, but that skill turns out to be something where speed of acquisition is not at all important. … We don’t say that someone who learned to walk at four months is a better walker than the rest of us. It’s not really a meaningful category.”

    Reading may be like walking in this respect. Gladwell cited one study comparing French-speaking Swiss children, who are taught to read early, with German-speaking Swiss children, who are taught to read later but show far fewer learning problems than their French-speaking counterparts; he also mentioned other research finding little if any correlation between early reading and ease or love of reading at later ages.

    When we call a child “precocious,” Gladwell said, “we have a very sloppy definition of what we mean. Generally what we mean is that a person has an unusual level of intellectual ability for their age.” But adult success has to do with a lot more than that. “In our obsession with precociousness we are overstating the importance of being smart.” In this regard, Gladwell noted research by Carol Dweck and Martin Seligman indicating that different dimensions such as explanatory styles and attitudes and approaches to learning may have as much to do with learning ability as does innate intelligence. And when it comes to musicians, the strongest predictor of ability is the same mundane thing that gets you to Carnegie Hall: “Really what we mean … when we say that someone is ‘naturally gifted’ is that they practice a lot, that they want to practice a lot, that they like to practice a lot.”

    So what about the ur-child-prodigy, Mozart? Famously, Mozart started to compose music at age four; by six, he is supposed to have traveled around Europe giving special performances with his father, Leopold. “He is of course the great poster child for precociousness,” Gladwell said. “More Upper West Side adults have pointed to Mozart, I’m quite sure, as a justification for sending their kids to excruciating early music programs, than almost any other historical figure.”

    Yet Gladwell deftly debunked the Mozart myth. “First of all, the music he composes at four isn’t any good,” he stated bluntly. “They’re basically arrangements of works by other composers. And also, rather suspiciously, they’re written down by his father. … And Leopold, it must be clear, is the 18th-century equivalent of a little league father.” Indeed Wolfgang’s storied performing precocity was exaggerated somewhat by his father’s probable lying about his age. (“Mozart was the Danny Almonte of his time,” Gladwell quipped, referring to the Bronx little league pitcher whose perfect game in 2001 was thrown out of the record books when it was revealed that he was 14, not 12, and thus too old for little league.)

    But most importantly, the young Mozart’s prowess can be chalked up to practice, practice, practice. Compelled to practice three hours a day from age three on, by age six the young Wolfgang had logged an astonishing 3,500 hours — “three times more than anybody else in his peer group. No wonder they thought he was a genius.” So Mozart’s famous precociousness as a musician was not innate musical ability but rather his ability to work hard, and circumstances (i.e., his father) that pushed him to do so.

    “That is a very different definition of precociousness than I think the one that we generally deal with.”

    A better poster child for what precociousness really entails, Gladwell hinted, may thus be the famous intellectual late-bloomer, Einstein. Gladwell cited a biographer’s description of the future physicist, who displayed no remarkable native intelligence as a child but whose success seems to have derived from certain habits and personality traits — curiosity, doggedness, determinedness — that are the less glamorous but perhaps more essential components of genius.

    Precocious is Pernicious

    Our romanticized view of precociousness matters. When certain kids are singled out as gifted or talented, Gladwell suggested, it creates an environment that may be subtly discouraging to those who are just average. “In singling out people like me at age 13 for special treatment, we discouraged other kids from ever taking up running at all. And we will never know how many kids who might have been great milers had they been encouraged and not discouraged from joining running, might have ended up as being very successful 10 years down the road.”

    Although Gladwell acknowledged the wisdom of wanting to provide learning environments suited to different paces of achievement, he suggested that “that very worthy goal is overwhelmed by … our irresistible desire to look at precociousness as a prediction.”

    “We thought that Doug Consiglio was a runner without talent,” he said, returning to his earlier example. “But what if he just didn’t take running seriously until he was 16 or 17? What if he suddenly found a coach who inspired him?” Predictions from childhood about adult performance can only be made based on relatively fixed traits, he said. “Unfortunately … many of the things that really matter in predicting adult success are not fixed at all. And once you begin to concede the importance of these kinds of non-intellectual, highly variable traits, you have to give up your love of precociousness.”

    Gladwell concluded his talk with a story he said his brother, an elementary school principal, likes to tell — “the story of two buildings. One is built ahead of schedule, and one is being built in New York City and comes in two years late and several million dollars over budget. Does anyone really care, 10 years down the road, which building was built early and which building was built late? … But somehow I think when it comes to children we feel the other way, that we get obsessed with schedules, and not with buildings. I think that’s a shame. … If you want to know whether a 13-year-old runner will be a good runner when they’re 23, you should wait until they’re 23.”

  • Again, it depends on what activities we're talking about. Where constructive work is the single most important factor in predicting performance, as may be the case in athletics, it will obviously be true, all else being equal. And maybe, up to a certain level, it's also true of chess, I wouldn't know. It may even be true, to a large extent, for classical musical performers.

    But consider that training requirements are not equally well-understood in all fields. What's required of a would-be runner or a concert pianist is far better understood than what kind of work or practice is required of a would-be novelist, composer or poet. And the failure, or inability, to codify the training program required may itself be proof of the counter-argument: work alone can't explain results, if the training required isn't well understood, despite practice of the art form for thousands of years. The point being, the failure to codify the training program is itself proof that training doesn't explain performance.

    Or try it this way: the fact that you can teach a 7-year old to play a very creditable game of chess or give a decent performance of a Mozart piano sonata is itself proof that instruction and work are very powerful, on their own. But there's no way to teach a 7-year old to write a good poem, novel or piano sonata. The activities are fundamentally different.

  • @JRD

    Or try it this way: the fact that you can teach a 7-year old to play a very creditable game of chess or give a decent performance of a Mozart piano sonata is itself proof that instruction and work are very powerful, on their own. But there's no way to teach a 7-year old to write a good poem, novel or piano sonata. The activities are fundamentally different.

    Right, it's the difference between technical acumen and creativity. It's what you called "Constructive work"?

    Maybe we're focusing too much on little kids. Labels are dangerous, that my daughter has been labelled as "Musically gifted" is probably a bad thing. It creates expectations, vanity, and worst of all, bragging on the part of goofy parents like me. It's a failure warrant in some ways. I try and tell her "You're talented, but so are a lot of other kids, you won't get anywhere without work." as Vitaliy said, brains are structurally malleable, via repetition and practice.

    So can creativity be acquired? I still see no reason why not -- though expecting it to show up in 6 year old is a tough sell perhaps. There are codified training systems too -- want to write a great poem? sit down in front of a blank computer screen and open a vein. Lather. Rinse. Repeat.